Recursiveness as the Dominant Feature of Communicative Behavior in Day-to-Day Interaction

Cover Page

Cite item

Abstract

Day-to-day interaction is overtly standardized and being associated with the expectations and norms imposed by a society as well as stereotypical societal being. When communicating, an individual is perfectly aware of the roles that could be attributed to him and aims to stand up to them. At the same time he intends to promptly comprehend and process the data he obtains from the outside and fit them into his niche, or the hierarchical environment determined by his unique set of properties. To put it differently, the subject of communication reflects upon those elements which are relevant for his being, and he can comprehend given his organization. With this it makes sense to relate to repetitive nature of some actions that have proven instrumental in maintaining the hierarchy of this environment and ensure self-preservation of the subject. Chilean biologists Humberto Maturana and Francisco Varela have coined the term recursiveness to relate to the repetitive character of the activities of a living entity in different cases of this living entity’s interaction with is medium. The authors of the theory of autopoiesis mainly focused on biological species, and humans being one of those. In the publications while they cover the foundations of their theory, Humberto Maturana and Francisco Varela refer to social systems as complex third order systems being regulated by the same self-preservation laws as a living entity. This serves as a prerequisite to attribute recursiveness to communicative behavior of a human aiming to step into social interaction and design a model of recursive communicative behavior. To verify this model we planned and ran a psycholinguistic experiment to measure the degree of the steoretypization in communicative behavior of mainstream representatives of the society. The participants were subdivided into two contrasting subclusters: “those under 24” and “those over 45”. They were offered a number of experimental cases with a task to model the communicative behavioral patterns typical for them in various day-to-day communication settings. Overall the experiment involved 219 subjects. The analysis of the data obtained outlines those settings implying a higher degree of the standardization in communicative behavior as well as the gender and age-related specifics of the participants with more stereotypical speech patterns.

About the authors

Vera A. Pishchalnikova

Moscow State Linguistic University

Email: pishchalnikova@mail.ru
ORCID iD: 0000-0002-0992-0466
SPIN-code: 2480-2244
Scopus Author ID: 57214930556
ResearcherId: HKN-8152-2023

D.Sc. in Philology, Full Professor, Professor with the Chair of general and comparative linguistics

38, Ostozhenka Str., Bld. 1, Moscow, Russian Federation, 119034

Ksenya S. Kardanova-Biryukova

Moscow City University

Author for correspondence.
Email: kardanova-biryukovaks@mgpu.ru
ORCID iD: 0000-0002-6773-1129
SPIN-code: 4523-4210
Scopus Author ID: 57205289987
ResearcherId: M-8605-2018

D.Sc. in Philology, Associate Professor, Head of the Chair of linguistics and translation studies with the Institute of foreign languages

4, 2nd Sel’skokhozyaystvenny proezd, Bld. 1, Moscow, Russian Federation, 129226

References

  1. Maturana, H.R. (2014). Understanding social systems? Constructivist Foundations, 9(2), 187–188.
  2. Maturana, H.R. & Varela, Fr. (1980). Autopoiesis and Cognition: The Realization of the Living. Boston Studies in the Philosophy of Science. Vol. 42. Dordecht: D. Reidel Publishing Co.
  3. Maturana, H.R. (1987). The biological foundation of self-consciousness and the physical domain of existence. In: Physics of cognitive Processes. World Scientific. Singapore. pp. 324–379.
  4. Maturana, H.R. (1995). Biology of Cognition. In: Language and Intellect. Moscow: Progress. pp. 95–142. (In Russ.).
  5. Maturana, H.R. (1978). Biology of Language: the Epistemology of Reality. In: Psychology and Biology of Language and Thought: Essays in Honor of Eric Lenneberg, Miller G.A., Lenneberg E. (eds.). New York: Academic Press. pp. 27–63.
  6. Luhmann, N. (2007). Social Systems. Saint-Petersburg: Nauka. (In Russ.).
  7. Parsons, T. (2008). Social Systems. Issues of Social Theory, II 1(2), 38–71. (In Russ.).
  8. Urrestarazu, H. (2014). Social autopoiesis? Constructivist Foundations, 9(2), 153–166.
  9. Luhmann, N. (2004). Society as a Social System. Moscow: Logos. (In Russ.).
  10. Shchus, A. & Parsons, T. (2021). Theory of Social Action. Moscow: Elementarny formy. (In Russ.).
  11. Goffman, E. (1959). The Presentation of Self in Everyday Life. New York: Doubleday: Garden City.
  12. Brown, P. & Levinson, S.C. (1987). Politeness: Some universals in language usage. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
  13. Cialdini, R. (1984). Influence. The Psychology of Persuasion. New York: William Morrow & Company.
  14. Gumperz, J. (1975). Types of Language Communities. In: New Issues of Linguistics. Iss. VII. Sociolinguistics. Moscow. pp. 182–198. (In Russ.).
  15. Maturana, H.R. & Varela, Fr. (2001). The Tree of Knowledge. Moscow: Progress. (In Russ.).
  16. Maturana, H.R. & Varela, Fr. (1980). Biology of cognition. In: Autopoiesis and Cognition: The Realization of the Living. Boston Studies in the Philosophy of Science. Vol. 42. Dordecht: D. Reidel Publishing Co. pp. 5–58.
  17. Damasio, A.R. (1994). Descartes’ Error: Emotion, Reason and the Human Brain. New York: Avan Books.
  18. Edelman, G.M. & Totoni, G. (2000). A Universe of Consciousness. How Matter Becomes Imagination. New York: Basic Books.
  19. Zelazo, P.D., Gao, H.H. & Todd, R. (2007). The development of consciousness. In: The Cambridge handbook of consciousness, P.D. Zelazo, M. Moscovitch, E. Thompson (eds.). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. pp. 405–432. https://doi.org/10.1017/ CBO9780511816789.016
  20. Totaro, P. & Ninno, D. (2018). A duality in recursion to analyse digital society. Advance. Preprint. https://doi.org/10.31124/advance.7228325.v1
  21. Rubin, S. (2023). Cartography of the multiple formal systems of molecular autopoiesis: from the biology of cognition and enaction to anticipation and active inference. Biosystems, 230. URL: https://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S0303264723001302 (accessed: 06.07.2023). https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biosystems.2023.104955
  22. Pishchal’nikova, V.A., Stepykin, N.I., Bubnova, I.A., Adamova, Z.G., Myagkova, E.Yu., Kruzhilina, T.V. & Butakova, L.O. Functional Illiteracy as Object of Psycholinguistics (2022). Moscow: R-Valent. (In Russ.).
  23. Stepykin, N.I. (2021). Potential in Using Formalized Parameters for Modeling Speech Actions. Scientific Dialogue, 8, 112–127. https://doi.org/10.24224/2227-1295-2021-8-112127 (In Russ.).
  24. Trofimova, I.N. (2004). Synergy of Body Dynamics and World Perception. In: Synergetics and Psychology. Moscow: Cognito-center. pp. 61–81. (In Russ.).

Supplementary files

Supplementary Files
Action
1. Fig. 1. Model of Recursive Communicative Behaviour of a Social Individual

Download (173KB)
2. Fig. 2. Polarization of the Answers by the Experiment Participants with Similar Gender and Age Parameters

Download (252KB)

Copyright (c) 2023 Pishchalnikova V.A., Kardanova-Biryukova K.S.

Creative Commons License
This work is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution-NonCommercial 4.0 International License.

This website uses cookies

You consent to our cookies if you continue to use our website.

About Cookies