Social Competence of Adolescents: The Role of Social Cognition and Behavior Control

Cover Page

Cite item

Abstract

Social competence during adolescence plays a crucial role in socialization and psychological well-being. Available data suggest that social cognition (theory of mind, ToM) and self-regulation (behavioral control) abilities make a significant contribution to social competence at a young age, but research on their role in adolescence is still limited. The purpose of the research is to study the role of behavior control and ToM in the social competence of adolescents. The study involved 106 6-8-grade students (girls - 51), aged 12-15 years ( Me = 13, SD = 0.87). The BRIEF questionnaire was used to assess their behavior control; ToM was assessed using tasks for understanding high-order false beliefs. For external assessment of the social competence of the participants, a questionnaire for teachers and a sociometric method were used. The groups of the adolescents, divided by the median criterion, were compared using the overall BRIEF regulation index and the total score for false-belief understanding. The most salient differences in the teachers’ and peers’ assessments of the social competence were found between the contrasting groups of the participants, i.e., with high levels of ToM and behavior control and with low levels of these abilities. At the same time, differences in the teachers’ assessments were observed for both positive and negative assessments but, in sociometric indices, only for negative ones. The teachers considered the adolescents with high levels of behavior control to be more socially competent, regardless of their level of social cognition. For the peers, the level of behavioral control mattered only when the ability to assess the mental states of other people was weak. The results of our study show the importance of the role of social cognition and behavioral control in the implementation of socially competent behavior in adolescents. At the same time, the data obtained indicate the heterogeneity of the contribution of these abilities to the social competence of adolescents assessed by peers and teachers.

About the authors

Galina A. Vilenskaya

Institute of Psychology of the Russian Academy of Sciences

Author for correspondence.
Email: vga2001@mail.ru
ORCID iD: 0000-0002-6089-155X

Ph.D. in Psychology, is Leading Researcher, Laboratory of Psychology of Subject Development in Normal and Post-Traumatic Conditions

13 Yaroslavskaya St, bldg 1, Moscow, 129366, Russian Federation

Evgenia I. Lebedeva

Institute of Psychology of the Russian Academy of Sciences

Email: evlebedeva@yandex.ru
ORCID iD: 0000-0002-0888-8273

Ph.D. in Psychology, is Senior Researcher, Laboratory of Psychology of Subject Development in Normal and Post-Traumatic States

13 Yaroslavskaya St, bldg 1, Moscow, 129366, Russian Federation

Anna Yu. Ulanova

Institute of Psychology of the Russian Academy of Sciences

Email: rachugina@gmail.com
ORCID iD: 0000-0002-8757-0916

Ph.D. in Psychology, is Senior Researcher, Laboratory of Psychology of Subject Development in Normal and Post-Traumatic States

13 Yaroslavskaya St, bldg 1, Moscow, 129366, Russian Federation

References

  1. Anderson, P.J. (2008). Towards a developmental model of executive function. In V. Anderson, R. Jacobs & P.J. Anderson (Eds.), Executive Functions and the Frontal Lobes: A Lifespan Perspective (pp. 3–21). New York: Psychology Press. https://doi.org/10.4324/9780203837863
  2. Argyle, M. (1999). Causes and correlates of happiness. In D. Kahneman, E. Diener & N. Schwarz (Eds.), Well-Being: The Foundations of Hedonic Psychology (pp. 353–373). New York: Russell Sage Foundation.
  3. Belickaja, G.Je. (1995). Social competence of the person. In K.A. Abulhanova-Slavskaja, A.V. Brushlinskij & M.I. Volovikova (Eds.), Consciousness of the Person in a Crisis Society (pp. 42–57). Moscow: Institute of Psychology of RAS. (In Russ.)
  4. Bierman, K.L., Nix, R.L., Greenberg, M.T., Blair, C., & Domitrovich, C.E. (2008). Executive functions and school readiness intervention: Impact, moderation, and mediation in the Head Start REDI program. Development and Psychopathology, 20(3), 821–843. https://doi.org/10.1017/s0954579408000394
  5. Bock, A.M., Gallaway, K.C., & Hund, A.M. (2015). Specifying links between executive functioning and theory of mind during middle childhood: Cognitive flexibility predicts social understanding. Journal of Cognition and Development, 16(3), 509–521. https://doi.org/10.1080/15248372.2014.888350
  6. Bosacki, S., & Astington, J.W. (2001). Theory of mind in preadolescence: Relations between social understanding and social competence. Social Development, 8(2), 237–255. https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-9507.00093
  7. Clark, C., Prior, M., & Kinsella, G. (2002). The relationship between executive function abilities, adaptive behaviour, and academic achievement in children with externalising behaviour problems. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 43(6), 785–796. https://doi.org/10.1111/1469-7610.00084
  8. Crick, N.R., & Dodge, K.A. (1994). A review and reformulation of social information-processing mechanisms in children's social adjustment. Psychological Bulletin, 115(1), 74–101. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.115.1.74
  9. Devine, R.T., White, N., Ensor, R., & Hughes, C. (2016). Theory of mind in middle childhood: Longitudinal associations with executive function and social competence. Developmental Psychology, 52(5), 758–771. https://doi.org/10.1037/dev0000105
  10. Dubrovina, O.I. (2009). Psychological foundations of professional and social competencies. PhD Thesis Abstract. Ekaterinburg: Ural State University. (In Russ.)
  11. Ferguson, H.J., Brunsdon, V.E.A., & Bradford, E.E.F. (2021). The developmental trajectories of executive function from adolescence to old age. Scientific Reports, 11(1), 1382. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-020-80866-1
  12. Gartvik, Ye.V. (2020). The ratio of the mental and delinquent model of adolescent behaviour. Vestnik of Kostroma State University. Series: Pedagogy. Psychology. Sociokinetics, 26(3), 73–78. (In Russ.) https://doi.org/10.34216/2073-1426-2020-26-3-73-78
  13. Gioia, G.A., Isquith, P.K., Guy, S.C., & Kenworthy, L. (2000). BRIEF: Behavior Rating Inventory of Executive Function. Lutz, FL: Psychological Assessment Resources, Inc.
  14. Gomez-Garibello, C., & Talwar, V. (2015). Can you read my mind? Age as a moderator in the relationship between theory of mind and relational aggression. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 39(6), 552–559. https://doi.org/10.1177/0165025415580805
  15. Gorina, E.Ju., & Ahutina, T.V. (Eds.). (2019). BRIEF Behavior Rating Inventory. Moscow: Terevinf Publ. (In Russ.)
  16. Imuta, K., Henry, J.D., Slaughter, V., Selcuk, B., & Ruffman, T. (2016). Theory of mind and prosocial behavior in childhood: A meta-analytic review. Developmental Psychology, 52(8), 1192–1205. https://doi.org/10.1037/dev0000140
  17. Isquith, P.K., Roth, R.M., Kenworthy, L., & Gioia, G. (2014). Contribution of rating scales to intervention for executive dysfunction. Applied Neuropsychology: Child, 3(3), 197–204. https://doi.org/10.1080/21622965.2013.870014
  18. Ivaniushina, V.A., Titkova, V.V., & Alexandrov, D.A. (2016). Adolescent aggression: Group norms and social status among peers. Sociological Journal, 22(1), 54–71. (In Russ.) https://doi.org/10.19181/socjour.2016.22.1.1294
  19. Jacobson, L.A., Williford, A.P., & Pianta, R.C. (2011). The role of executive function in children's competent adjustment to middle school. Child Neuropsychology, 17(3), 255–280. https://doi.org/10.1080/09297049.2010.535654
  20. Junge, C., Valkenburg, P.M., Deković, M., & Branje, S. (2020). The building blocks of social competence: Contributions of the consortium of individual development. Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience, 45, 100861. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.dcn.2020.100861
  21. Laursen, B., & Veenstra, R. (2021). Toward understanding the functions of peer influence: A summary and synthesis of recent empirical research. Journal of Research on Adolescence, 31(4), 889–907. https://doi.org/10.1111/jora.12606
  22. Lebedeva, E.I., Vilenskaya, G.A., Ulanova, A.Yu., Filippu, O.Yu., & Pavlova, N.S. (2023). Social competence in adolescents with different level of theory of mind. Psikhologicheskii Zhurnal, 44(1), 43–52. (In Russ.) https://doi.org/10.31857/S020595920024342-2
  23. Liddle, B., & Nettle, D. (2006). Higher-order theory of mind and social competence in school-age children. Journal of Cultural and Evolutionary Psychology, 4(3‒4), 231–244. https://doi.org/10.1556/jcep.4.2006.3-4.3
  24. Maslova, I.A. (2007). Socialization of an adolescent in the educational space of an institution of additional education for children. Ph.D. in Pedagogy Thesis Abstract. Magnitogorsk: Magnitogorsk State Technical University. (In Russ.)
  25. Masten, A.S., Herbers, J.E., Desjardins, C.D., Cutuli, J.J., McCormick, C.M., Sapienza, J.K., Long, J.D., & Zelazo, P.D. (2012). Executive function skills and school success in young children experiencing homelessness. Educational Researcher, 41(9), 375–384. https://doi.org/10.3102/0013189x12459883
  26. Meinhardt‐Injac, B., Daum, M.M., & Meinhardt, G. (2020). Theory of mind development from adolescence to adulthood: Testing the two‐component model. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 38(2), 289–303. https://doi.org/10.1111/bjdp.12320
  27. Miller, S.E., Reavis, R.E., & Avila, B.N. (2018). Associations between theory of mind, executive function, and friendship quality in middle childhood. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 64(3), 397–426. https://doi.org/10.13110/merrpalmquar1982.64.3.0397
  28. O’Toole, S.E., Monks, C.P., & Tsermentseli, S. (2017). Executive function and theory of mind as predictors of aggressive and prosocial behavior and peer acceptance in early childhood. Social Development, 26(4), 907–920. https://doi.org/10.1111/sode.12231
  29. Raaijmakers, M.A.J., Smidts, D.P., Sergeant, J.A., Maassen, G.H., Posthumus, J.A., van Engeland, H., & Matthys, W. (2008). Executive functions in preschool children with aggressive behavior: Impairments in inhibitory control. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 36(7), 1097–1107. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10802-008-9235-7
  30. Razza, R.A., & Blair, C. (2009). Associations among false-belief understanding, executive function, and social competence: A longitudinal analysis. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology, 30(3), 332–343. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.appdev.2008.12.020
  31. Repacholi, B., Slaughter, V., Pritchard, M., & Gibbs, V. (2003). Theory of mind, Machiavellianism, and social functioning in childhood. In B. Repacholi & V. Slaughter (Eds.), Individual Differences in Theory of Mind: Implications for Typical and Atypical Development (pp. 67–97). New York: Psychology Press. https://doi.org/10.4324/9780203488508
  32. Rose-Krasnor, L. (1997). The nature of social competence: A theoretical review. Social Development, 6(1), 111–135. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9507.1997.tb00097.x
  33. Schultz, D., Izard, C.E., Stapleton, L.M., Buckingham-Howes, S., & Bear, G.A. (2009). Children's social status as a function of emotionality and attention control. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology, 30(2), 169–181. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.appdev.2008.12.002
  34. Sergienko, E.A. (2018). Behaviour control as the basis of self-regulation. South-Russian Journal of Social Sciences, 19(4), 130–146. (In Russ.) https://doi.org/10.31429/26190567-19-4-130-146
  35. Sergienko, E.A., & Vilenskaya, G.A. (2018). Behavioral control ‒ integrative concept of mental regulation. In A.L. Zhuravlev & E.A. Sergienko (Eds.), The Development of the Concepts of Contemporary Psychology (pp. 343–378). Moscow: Institute of Psychology of RAS. (In Russ.)
  36. Serjakova, S.B., & Galakova, O.V. (2013). Social competence of primary schoolchildren. Development in extracurricular activities. Saarbruecken: LAP Lambert Academic Publishing. (In Russ.)
  37. Slaughter, V., Imuta, K., Peterson, C.C., & Henry, J.D. (2015). Meta-analysis of theory of mind and peer popularity in the preschool and early school years. Child Development, 86(4), 1159–1174. https://doi.org/10.1111/cdev.12372
  38. Smith, P.K. (2017). Bullying and theory of mind: A review. Current Psychiatry Reviews, 13(2), 90–95. https://doi.org/10.2174/1573400513666170502123214
  39. Stump, K.N., Ratliff, J.M., Wu, Y.P., & Hawley, P.H. (2009). Theories of social competence from the top-down to the bottom-up: A case for considering foundational human needs. In J.L. Matson (Ed.), Social Behavior and Skills in Children (pp. 23–37). New York: Springer. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-1-4419-0234-4_2
  40. Titkova, V.V., Ivaniushina, V.A., & Alexandrov, D.A. (2017). Smart, pretty or independent: Who is popular at school? Educational Studies Moscow, (4), 171–198. (In Russ.) https://doi.org/10.17323/1814-9545-2017-4-171-198
  41. Van den Berg, Y.H.M., Lansu, T.A.M., & Cillessen, A.H.N. (2020). Preference and popularity as distinct forms of status: A meta‐analytic review of 20 years of research. Journal of Adolescence, 84(1), 78–95. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.adolescence.2020.07.010
  42. Van der Graaff, J., Carlo, G., Crocetti, E., Koot, H.M., & Branje, S. (2018). Prosocial behavior in adolescence: Gender differences in development and links with empathy. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 47(5), 1086–1099. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10964-017-0786-1
  43. Vilenskaya, G.A., & Lebedeva, E.I. (2014). The development of the understanding of mental world and the behavioural control in preschool age. Psychological Studies, 7(38), 5. (In Russ.) https://doi.org/10.54359/ps.v7i38.587
  44. Weimer, A.A., Warnell, K.R., Ettekal, I., Cartwright, K.B., Guajardo, N.R., & Liew, J. (2021). Correlates and antecedents of theory of mind development during middle childhood and adolescence: An integrated model. Developmental Review, 59, 100945. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.dr.2020.100945
  45. Wilson, J., Hogan, C., Wang, S., Andrews, G., & Shum, D.H.K. (2021). Relations between executive functions, theory of mind, and functional outcomes in middle childhood. Developmental Neuropsychology, 46(7), 518–536. https://doi.org/10.1080/87565641.2021.1988086
  46. Yudina, T.A., & Alekhina, S.V. (2022). On the problem of evidence-based psychological assessment of school students’ social competence in inclusive education. Journal of Modern Foreign Psychology, 11(4), 41–50. (In Russ.) https://doi.org/10.17759/jmfp.2022110404
  47. Zelazo, P.D., & Müller, U. (2002). Executive function in typical and atypical development. In U. Goswami (Ed.), Blackwell Handbook of Childhood Cognitive Development (pp. 445–469). Malden, MA: Blackwell Publishers Ltd. https://doi.org/10.1002/9780470996652.ch20
  48. Zorza, J.P., Marino, J., & Acosta Mesas, A. (2016). Executive functions as predictors of school performance and social relationships: Primary and secondary school students. The Spanish Journal of Psychology, 19, E23. https://doi.org/10.1017/sjp.2016.23
  49. Zorza, J.P., Marino, J., de Lemus, S., & Acosta Mesas, A. (2013). Academic performance and social competence of adolescents: Predictions based on effortful control and empathy. The Spanish Journal of Psychology, 16, E87. https://doi.org/10.1017/sjp.2013.87

Copyright (c) 2023 Vilenskaya G.A., Lebedeva E.I., Ulanova A.Y.

Creative Commons License
This work is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution-NonCommercial 4.0 International License.

This website uses cookies

You consent to our cookies if you continue to use our website.

About Cookies