Using Language Entropy to Characterize Bilingual Language Experience: a Study of Adyghe-Russian and Tatar-Russian Bilinguals


Cite item

Abstract

It is believed that bilingualism contributes to the enhancement of executive functions in bilingual individuals as they need to constantly control the simultaneous activation of two or more languages in their brains. However, decades of research have led to contradictory conclusions regarding the existence of bilingual advantage. One of the promising approaches to resolving these contradictions is a more detailed examination of bilingual language experience. The aim of this study was twofold. First is to empirically test the language entropy method as a way of measuring variability in bilingual language use in different interaction contexts. Second is to examine the relationship between bilinguals’ language entropy and executive functions (cognitive flexibility, goal maintenance, conflict monitoring). The study involved 111 bilinguals (mean age = 20.5 (2.97); 75.7% female), who are speakers of Adyghe-Russian and Tatar-Russian languages. The battery of instruments included a questionnaire on demographic and language experience, containing questions to calculate language entropy in four contexts (home, university, work, free time), a Color-shape switching task to measure executive functions (domains of cognitive flexibility, goal updating, conflict monitoring), and Cattell Culture Fair Intelligence Test to assess nonverbal intelligence. The results demonstrated that language entropy of the bilinguals from both regions did not exceed a score of 0.66 in all the contexts which indicates the use of predominantly one language in everyday communications. However, in the “university” context, bilinguals speaking Tatar and Russian used languages in a more balanced way than bilinguals speaking Adyghe and Russian languages. The results suggest that the language entropy method can be highly effective for characterizing bilingual language experience. The results also showed that there was no relationship between language entropy of the bilinguals in this study and their executive functions (domains of cognitive flexibility, goal updating, conflict monitoring). This is consistent with the predictions of the Adaptive Control Hypothesis.

About the authors

Elena Yu. Semenova

Sirius University of Science and Technology

Author for correspondence.
Email: esem7enova@gmail.com
ORCID iD: 0000-0003-0252-6295
SPIN-code: 4283-0755

Junior Researcher, Center for Cognitive Sciences

1 Olympiysky Ave., Sirius, Federal Territory “Sirius”, Krasnodar Territory, 354340, Russian Federation

Katerina V. Lind

Sirius University of Science and Technology

Email: lind.kv@talantiuspeh.ru
ORCID iD: 0009-0004-8447-0452

PhD in Population Health, Research Fellow, Center for Cognitive Sciences

1 Olympiysky Ave., Sirius, Federal Territory “Sirius”, Krasnodar Territory, 354340, Russian Federation

Tatiana I. Logvinenko

Ludwig-Maximilians-Universitat Munich

Email: logvinenkota.spb@gmail.com
ORCID iD: 0000-0001-7430-1963
SPIN-code: 8068-9856

Research Fellow, Department of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, Psychosomatics and Psychotherapy, University Hospital

Geschwister-Scholl-Platz 1, 80539 Munich, Germany

Elena L. Grigorenko

Sirius University of Science and Technology; Moscow State University of Psychology and Education; University of Houston; Baylor College of Medicine; Yale University

Email: Elena.Grigorenko@times.uh.edu
ORCID iD: 0000-0001-9646-4181
SPIN-code: 4453-4618

PhD in General and Educational Psychology, PhD in Developmental Psychology and Genetics, Scientific Supervisor, Sirius Center for Cognitive Sciences, Sirius University of Science and Technology (Sirius, Krasnodar Region, Russia); Leading Scientist, Moscow State University of Psychology and Education (Moscow, Russia); Hugh Roy and Lillie Cranz Cullen Distinguished Professor of Psychology, University of Houston (Houston, TX, USA); Professor, Baylor College of Medicine (Houston, TX, USA); Professor, Yale University (New Haven, CT, USA). Honorary Editor of RUDN Journal of Psychology and Pedagogics

1 Olympiysky Ave., Sirius, Federal Territory “Sirius”, Krasnodar Territory, 354340, Russian Federation; 2 Karetny Ryad, Moscow, 127051, Russian Federation; 4300 Martin Luther King Blvd, Houston, TX, 77204, USA; 1 Baylor Plz, Houston, TX, 77030, USA; 230 South Frontage Road, New Haven, CT, 06518, USA

References

  1. Alrwaita, N., Houston-Price, C., Meteyard, L., Voits, T., & Pliatsikas, C. (2024). Executive functions are modulated by the context of dual language use: Diglossic, bilingual and monolingual older adults. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 27(1), 178-203. https://doi.org/10.1017/S1366728923000056
  2. Anderson, J. A. E., Mak, L., Keyvani Chahi, A., & Bialystok, E. (2018). The language and social background questionnaire: Assessing degree of bilingualism in a diverse population. Behavior Research Methods, 50(1), 250-263. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13428-017-0867-9
  3. Bates, D., Maechler, M., Bolker, B., Walker, S., Christensen, R. H. B., Singmann, H., ... & Bolker, M. B. (2015). Package ‘lme4’. Convergence, 12(1), 2.
  4. Beatty-Martínez, A.L., Navarro-Torres, C.A., Dussias, P.E., Bajo, M.T., Guzzardo Tamargo, R.E., & Kroll, J.F. (2020). Interactional context mediates the consequences of bilingualism for language and cognition. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 46(6), 1022-1047. https://doi.org/10.1037/xlm0000770
  5. Bialystok, E. (2001). Bilingualism in development: Language, literacy, and cognition. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
  6. Cattell, R. B., & Cattell, A. K. (1973). Measuring intelligence with the culture fair tests. Champaign, IL: Institute for Personality and Ability Testing.
  7. de Bruin, A. (2019). Not all bilinguals are the same: A call for more detailed assessments and descriptions of bilingual experiences. Behavioral Sciences, 9(3), 33. https://doi.org/10.3390/bs9030033
  8. DeLuca, V., Rothman, J., Bialystok, E., & Pliatsikas, C. (2019). Redefining bilingualism as a spectrum of experiences that differentially affects brain structure and function. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 116(15), 7565-7574. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1811513116
  9. Dick, A. S., Garcia, N. L., Pruden, S. M., Thompson, W. K., Hawes, S. W., Sutherland, M. T., Riedel, M. C., Laird, A. R., & Gonzalez, R. (2019). No evidence for a bilingual executive function advantage in the ABCD study. Nature Human Behaviour, 3(7), 692-701. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41562-019-0609-3
  10. Freeman, M., Anthony, J., Marian, V., & Blumenfeld, H. (2022). Individual and sociolinguistic differences in language background predict stroop performance. Frontiers in Communication, 7, 865965. https://doi.org/10.3389/fcomm.2022.865965
  11. Giovannoli, J., Martella, D., Federico, F., Pirchio, S., & Casagrande, M. (2020). The impact of bilingualism on executive functions in children and adolescents: A systematic review based on the PRISMA method. Frontiers in Psychology, 11, 574789. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2020.574789
  12. Green, D. W., & Abutalebi, J. (2013). Language control in bilinguals: The adaptive control hypothesis. Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 25(5), 515-530. https://doi.org/10.1080/20445911.2013.796377
  13. Grosjean, F. (2022). The Mysteries of Bilingualism: Unresolved Issues. Wiley.
  14. Grundy, J. G., Pavlenko, E., & Bialystok, E. (2020). Bilingualism modifies disengagement of attention networks across the scalp: A multivariate ERP investigation of the IOR paradigm. Journal of Neurolinguistics, 56, 100933. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jneuroling.2020.100933
  15. Gullifer, J. W., Chai, X. J., Whitford, V., Pivneva, I., Baum, S., Klein, D., & Titone, D. (2018). Bilingual experience and resting-state brain connectivity: Impacts of L2 age of acquisition and social diversity of language use on control networks. Neuropsychologia, 117, 123-134. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2018.04.037
  16. Gullifer, J. W., Pivneva, I., Whitford, V., Sheikh, N. A., & Titone, D. (2023). Bilingual language experience and its effect on conflict adaptation in reactive inhibitory control tasks. Psychological Science, 34(2), 238-251. https://doi.org/10.1177/09567976221113764
  17. Gullifer, J. W., & Titone, D. (2020). Characterizing the social diversity of bilingualism using language entropy. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 23(2), 283-294. https://doi.org/10.1017/S1366728919000026
  18. Gullifer, J. W., & Titone, D. (2021). Engaging proactive control: Influences of diverse language experiences using insights from machine learning. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 150(3), 414-430. https://doi.org/10.1037/xge0000933
  19. Gunnerud, H. L., Ten Braak, D., Reikerås, E. K. L., Donolato, E., & Melby-Lervåg, M. (2020). Is bilingualism related to a cognitive advantage in children? A systematic review and meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 146(12), 1059-1083. https://doi.org/10.1037/bul0000301
  20. Hartanto, A., & Yang, H. (2016). Disparate bilingual experiences modulate task-switching advantages: A diffusion-model analysis of the effects of interactional context on switch costs. Cognition, 150, 10-19. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2016.01.016
  21. Hartanto, A., & Yang, H. (2020). The role of bilingual interactional contexts in predicting interindividual variability in executive functions: A latent variable analysis. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 149(4), 609-633. https://psycnet.apa.org/doi/10.1037/xge0000672
  22. Huynh, Q.-L., Benet-Martínez, V., & Nguyen, A.-M. D. (2018). Measuring variations in bicultural identity across U.S. ethnic and generational groups: Development and validation of the Bicultural Identity Integration Scale-Version 2 (BIIS-2). Psychological Assessment, 30(12), 1581-1596. https://doi.org/10.1037/pas0000606
  23. Kałamała, P., Szewczyk, J., Chuderski, A., Senderecka, M., & Wodniecka, Z. (2020). Patterns of bilingual language use and response inhibition: A test of the adaptive control hypothesis. Cognition, 204, 104373. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2020.104373
  24. Kałamała, P., Walther, J., Zhang, H., Diaz, M., Senderecka, M., & Wodniecka, Z. (2022). The use of a second language enhances the neural efficiency of inhibitory control: An ERP study. Bilingualism, 25(1), 163-180. Scopus. https://doi.org/10.1017/S1366728921000389
  25. Khotinets, V.Yu., & Salnova, S.A. (2020). Executive functions and their relationship with the development of Russian speech in bilingual and monolingual children. RUDN Journal of Psychology and Pedagogics, 17(3), 412-425. (In Russ.) http://doi.org/10.22363/2313-1683-2020-17-3-412-425
  26. Kremin, L. V., & Byers-Heinlein, K. (2021). Why not both? Rethinking categorical and continuous approaches to bilingualism. International Journal of Bilingualism, 25(6), 1560-1575. https://doi.org/10.1177/13670069211031986
  27. Lehtonen, M., Soveri, A., Laine, A., Järvenpää, J., De Bruin, A., & Antfolk, J. (2018). Is bilingualism associated with enhanced executive functioning in adults? A meta-analytic review. Psychological Bulletin, 144(4), 394-425. https://doi.org/10.1037/bul0000142
  28. Li, P., Zhang, F., Tsai, E., & Puls, B. (2014). Language history questionnaire (LHQ 2.0): A new dynamic web-based research tool. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 17(3), 673-680. https://doi.org/10.1017/S1366728913000606
  29. Li, X., Ng, K. K., Wong, J. J. Y., Lee, J. W., Zhou, J. H., & Yow, W. Q. (2021). Bilingual language entropy influences executive functions through functional connectivity and signal variability. Brain and Language, 222, 105026. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bandl.2021.105026
  30. Lowe, C. J., Cho, I., Goldsmith, S. F., & Morton, J. B. (2021). The bilingual advantage in children’s executive functioning is not related to language status: A meta-analytic review. Psychological Science, 32(7), 1115-1146. https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797621993108
  31. Luk, G. (2023). Justice and equity for whom? Reframing research on the “bilingual (dis)advantage.” Applied Psycholinguistics, 44(3), 301-315. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0142716422000339
  32. Marian, V., Blumenfeld, H. K., & Kaushanskaya, M. (2007). The Language Experience and Proficiency Questionnaire (LEAP-Q): Assessing Language Profiles in Bilinguals and Multilinguals. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 50(4), 940-967. https://doi.org/10.1044/1092-4388(2007/067)
  33. Martin-Rhee, M. M., & Bialystok, E. (2008). The development of two types of inhibitory control in monolingual and bilingual children. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 11(1), 81-93. https://doi.org/10.1017/S1366728907003227
  34. Miyake, A., & Friedman, N. P. (2012). The nature and organization of individual differences in executive functions: Four general conclusions. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 21(1), 8-14. https://doi.org/10.1177/0963721411429458
  35. Nikolaeva, E.I., & Vergunov, E.G. (2017). What are “executive functions” and their development in ontogenesis. Theoretical and Experimental Psychology, 10(2), 62-81. (In Russ.)
  36. Ooi, S. H., Goh, W. D., Sorace, A., & Bak, T. H. (2018). From Bilingualism to Bilingualisms: Bilingual experience in Edinburgh and Singapore affects attentional control differently. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 21(4), 867-879. https://doi.org/10.1017/S1366728918000020
  37. Paap, K. (2022). The Bilingual Advantage in Executive Functioning Hypothesis: How the debate provides insight into psychology’s replication crisis (1st ed.). Routledge. https://doi.org/10.4324/9781003308027
  38. Paap, K. R., Johnson, H. A., & Sawi, O. (2015). Bilingual advantages in executive functioning either do not exist or are restricted to very specific and undetermined circumstances. Cortex, 69, 265-278. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cortex.2015.04.014
  39. Paxton, J. L., Barch, D. M., Racine, C. A., & Braver, T. S. (2008). Cognitive control, goal maintenance, and prefrontal function in healthy aging. Cerebral Cortex, 18(5), 1010-1028. https://doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bhm135
  40. Peirce, J. W. (2007). PsychoPy - Psychophysics software in Python. Journal of Neuroscience Methods, 162(1-2), 8-13. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jneumeth.2006.11.017
  41. Pliatsikas, C., DeLuca, V., & Voits, T. (2020). The Many Shades of Bilingualism: Language Experiences Modulate Adaptations in Brain Structure. Language Learning, 70(S2), 133-149. https://doi.org/10.1111/lang.12386
  42. Poarch, G. J., & van Hell, J. G. (2012). Executive functions and inhibitory control in multilingual children: Evidence from second-language learners, bilinguals, and trilinguals. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 113(4), 535-551. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2012.06.013
  43. Prior, A., & MacWhinney, B. (2010). A bilingual advantage in task switching. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 13(2), 253-262. https://doi.org/10.1017/S1366728909990526
  44. R Core Team. (2013). R: A language and environment for statistical computing. R Foundation for Statistical Computing, Vienna, Austria. Retrieved from http://www.R-project.org/
  45. RStudio Team. (2020). RStudio: Integrated Development for R. Boston: RStudio, PBC. Retrieved from http://www.rstudio.com/
  46. Seçer, I. (2016). Skills of cognitive flexibility in monolingual and bilingual younger adults. The Journal of General Psychology, 143(3), 172-184. https://doi.org/10.1080/00221309.2016.1200530
  47. Shannon, C. E. (1948). A Mathematical Theory of Communication. Bell System Technical Journal, 27(3), 379-423. https://doi.org/10.1002/j.1538-7305.1948.tb01338.x
  48. Soveri, A., Rodriguez-Fornells, A., & Laine, M. (2011). Is there a relationship between language switching and executive functions in bilingualism? Introducing a within-group analysis approach. Frontiers in Psychology, 2, 183. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2011.00183
  49. Studenica, A., Laketa, A., Chrysochoou, E., Blakey, E., & Vivas, A. B. (2022). The influence of bilingualism on adolescent cognition: The roles of biculturalism, the bilingual profile, and linguistic similarity. Cognitive Development, 63, 101203. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cogdev.2022.101203
  50. Surrain, S., & Luk, G. (2019). Describing bilinguals: A systematic review of labels and descriptions used in the literature between 2005-2015. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 22(2), 401-415. https://doi.org/10.1017/S1366728917000682
  51. Van Den Berg, F., Brouwer, J., Tienkamp, T. B., Verhagen, J., & Keijzer, M. (2022). language entropy relates to behavioral and pupil indices of executive control in young adult bilinguals. Frontiers in Psychology, 13, 864763. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2022.864763
  52. Veraksa, A.N., Gavrilova, M.N., & Bukhalenkova, D.A. (2019). Relationship between indicators of speech development and executive functions in preschool children: Analysis of research. Psikhologicheskii Zhurnal, 40(3), 64-76. (In Russ.) http://doi.org/10.31857/S020595920004599-4
  53. Von Bastian, C. C., Souza, A. S., & Gade, M. (2016). No evidence for bilingual cognitive advantages: A test of four hypotheses. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 145(2), 246-258. https://doi.org/10.1037/xge0000120
  54. Wagner, D., Bekas, K., & Bialystok, E. (2023). Does language entropy shape cognitive performance? A Tale of Two Cities. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 26(5), 998-1008. https://doi.org/10.1017/S1366728923000202
  55. Wiseheart, M., Viswanathan, M., & Bialystok, E. (2016). Flexibility in task switching by monolinguals and bilinguals. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 19(1), 141-146. https://doi.org/10.1017/S1366728914000273
  56. Xie, Z., & Antolovic, K. (2022). Differential impacts of natural L2 immersion and intensive classroom L2 training on cognitive control. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 75(3), 550-562. https://doi.org/10.1177/17470218211040813
  57. Zhou, B., & Krott, A. (2018). Bilingualism enhances attentional control in non-verbal conflict tasks - evidence from ex-Gaussian analyses. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 21(1), 162-180. https://doi.org/10.1017/S1366728916000869

Copyright (c) 2024 Semenova E.Y., Lind K.V., Logvinenko T.I., Grigorenko E.L.

Creative Commons License
This work is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution-NonCommercial 4.0 International License.

This website uses cookies

You consent to our cookies if you continue to use our website.

About Cookies